statistics/구조방정식(AMOS를 중심으로)

매개된 조절효과, 조절된 매개효과

반찬이 2008. 12. 7. 20:11
매개된 조절효과, 조절된 매개효과
University of Colorado at Boulder
Vincent Y. Yzerbyt
Catholic University of Louvain at Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
Procedures for examining whether treatment effects on an outcome are mediated and/or moderated have
been well developed and are routinely applied. The mediation question focuses on the intervening
mechanism that produces the treatment effect. The moderation question focuses on factors that affect the
magnitude of the treatment effect. It is important to note that these two processes may be combined in
informative ways, such that moderation is mediated or mediation is moderated. Although some prior
literature has discussed these possibilities, their exact definitions and analytic procedures have not been
completely articulated. The purpose of this article is to define precisely both mediated moderation and
moderated mediation and provide analytic strategies for assessing each.
Keywords: mediation, moderation, mediated moderation, moderated mediation
There is by now a very large literature on the important topics
of mediation and moderation (e.g., Aiken & West, 1991; Baron &
Kenny, 1986; James & Brett, 1984; Judd & Kenny, 1981; Kenny,
Kashy, & Bolger, 1998; Kraemer, Wilson, Fairburn, & Agras,
2002; MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002)
and in general it is fair to say that the analytic strategies underlying
the assessment of each are well understood (even if they are still
developing). Both processes focus on a given treatment effect. The
issue of mediation addresses how that treatment effect is produced.
Mediational analyses attempt to identify the intermediary process
that leads from the manipulated independent variable to the out-
come or dependent variable. The issue of moderation focuses on
factors that influence the strength and/or direction of the relation
between the treatment variable and the dependent variable. Mod-
erational analyses attempt to identify individual difference or
contextual variables that strengthen and/or change the direction of
the relationship between the treatment variable and the dependent
variable.
In this growing literature, there have been occasional discus-
sions of how the two issues of mediation and moderation might
themselves be combined in theoretically interesting ways. Thus, in
some of the early classic papers on mediation and moderation,
James and Brett (1984) discuss moderated mediation, and Baron
and Kenny (1986) devote a page to both moderated mediation and
mediated moderation. More recently Wegener and Fabrigar (2000)
also discuss these two topics. However, all of these treatments are
relatively brief, and our perusal of the literature that has made
reference to one or the other of these topics suggests that they are
not clearly understood. Additionally, there exists no source that
comprehensively articulates the definition of these two processes
and the analytic models that underlie them.
It is our intention in the current article to accomplish this. In the
initial section, we clearly define both mediated moderation and
moderated mediation and provide examples of each. In the second
section, we lay out the analytic models that are used to examine
both mediated moderation and moderated mediation. As we show
there, both of these processes rely on the same underlying models
of the data, but both have different starting points and thus respond
to distinct research questions. There is, however, a fundamental
defining algebraic equality that underlies both processes. Accord-
ingly, while we explicate the analyses for each, our argument is
that these two processes are in some sense the flip sides of the
same coin. In the third section, we use hypothetical data from the
two examples developed in the initial section to illustrate both
processes. We conclude by further discussing how these processes
relate to each other and then by considering a variety of issues
which our discussion has raised.
Defining Terms and Illustrating Processes
Initially we define terms in an abstract, generalized case. Sub-
sequent to this, we provide more detailed substantive examples
that hopefully will make clear why both processes, mediated
moderation and moderated mediation, are theoretically important.
The abstract case initially focuses on four variables. First there
is the manipulated independent variable, X
i
. To keep things simple,
Dominique Muller, Université Paris Descartes, France; Charles M. Judd,
University of Colorado at Boulder; Vincent Y. Yzerbyt, Catholic Univer-
sity of Louvain at Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.
This work was supported by a Lavoisier fellowship from the Ministe`re
des Affaires étrange`res, France, awarded to Dominique Muller and by
National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01MH45049 awarded to
Charles M. Judd. We express our gratitude to Olivier Corneille, Olivier
Desrichard, Marcello Galucci, and David MacKinnon for their comments
on a draft of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Domin-
ique Muller, Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale, Université Paris-5 René
Descarte, 71, avenue Edouard Vaillant, 92774 Boulogne Billancourt Ce-
dex, France, or to Charles M. Judd, Department of Psychology, University
of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309-0345. E-mail: dominique.muller@univ-
paris5.fr or charles.judd@colorado.edu
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association
2005, Vol. 89, No. 6, 852–863
0022-3514/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.852
852
we assume it has only two values, indicating whether a participant
was in the treatment or control condition, and its values are
contrast coded (e.g., 1, 1 or .5, .5). We further assume that
participants have been randomly assigned to one of these two
levels, so that causal inferences can be made about the treatment
effect.
1
The second variable is the outcome variable, Y
i
, which is
some measured response of the participants, presumed to be af-
fected by the treatment. Next there is a mediating variable, Me
i
,
which is another measured response variable, also expected to be
affected by the treatment variable. The mediational hypothesis, to
be examined, is that this mediator is responsible for the causal
effect of X
i
on Y
i
. Finally, there is a potential moderating variable,
Mo
i
, which is either some stable individual difference variable,
assumed not to be affected by the treatment, or else some measure
of the context or situation under which the treatment is delivered.
We presume it is measured prior to the delivery of the treatment,
and therefore, given random assignment to X
i
, it is assumed that
Mo
i
and X
i
are independent of each other.
2
In the general case, we
assume that this moderator is continuously measured, although
what we have to say also applies with a dichotomous, and hence
contrast-coded, moderator. The moderational hypothesis, to be
examined, is that the magnitude of the causal effect of X
i
on Y
i
depends on the moderator.
It is important to note that, throughout our discussion (unless
explicitly stated otherwise), we are presuming that all variables
(with the exception of the outcome) have been centered at their
mean. This assumption is made to increase the interpretability of
various parameters in models that include interaction terms (Aiken
& West, 1991; Judd & McClelland, 1989).
3
Although there is some continuing discussion about the neces-
sary and sufficient conditions to establish mediation (see below),
we have chosen to adopt the original and classic approach as
outlined by Judd and Kenny (1981) and Baron and Kenny (1986).
According to this approach, to demonstrate mediation, one esti-
mates three different models (typically using least squares
estimators)
4
Y
10
11
X
1
(1)
Me
20
21
X
2
(2)
Y
30
31
X
32
Me
3
(3)
and four conditions must be met:
1. In Equation 1, there must be an overall treatment effect
on the outcome variable; that is, b
11
is significant.
5
2. In Equation 2, there must be a treatment effect on the
mediator; that is, b
21
is significant.
3. In Equation 3, there must be an effect of the mediator on
the outcome controlling for the treatment; that is, b
32
is
significant.
4. In Equation 3, the residual direct effect of the treatment
variable on the outcome (
31
) should be smaller (in
absolute value) than the overall treatment effect in Equa-
tion 1 (
11
).
The following equality relationship exists among the parameters
of these models (see, e.g., MacKinnon, Warsi, & Dwyer, 1995):
11
31
21
*
32
meaning that the difference between the overall treatment effect
and the residual direct effect is equal to what is called the indirect
effect via the mediator (i.e.,
21
*
32
).
A continuing issue in the literature has focused on how one
establishes Condition 4. Alternatives here, outlined by MacKinnon
et al. (2002), involve testing whether the parameter difference on
the left side of the above equality departs from zero or whether the
product on the right side does so. An additional issue that contin-
ues to be debated concerns the necessity of Condition 1 (again,
MacKinnon et al., 2002; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). We agree here
with Shrout and Bolger (2002) who acknowledge that “experimen-
talists who wish to elaborate the mechanisms of an experimental
effect need to first establish that the effect exists” (p. 430). We do,
however, recognize that there may be grounds for disagreement here.
To demonstrate moderation, one estimates the following model:
Y
40
41
X
42
Mo
43
XMo
4
(4)
where XMo is computed as the product of the treatment variable
and the moderating variable. A test of the effect of that partialled
product (i.e., the significance of b
43
) is a test of the Treatment
Moderator interaction, asking whether the treatment effect varies
in magnitude as a function of the value of the moderator.
In this context, having defined the relevant variables and the
meaning of mediation and moderation, we are now able to define
the two processes of central interest to this article: mediated
moderation and moderated mediation. To clearly differentiate be-
tween them, we will define them in the most prototypic case,
clearly contrasting the two. Subsequently, we will examine their
interrelations and how one bleeds into the other.
The first of these, mediated moderation, can happen only when
moderation occurs: the magnitude of the overall treatment effect
on the outcome depends on the moderator. Given that the magni-
tude of the treatment effect depends on an individual difference or
context variable, then the mediated moderation question is con-
cerned with the mediating process that is responsible for that
moderation. What is the process through which that overall mod-
erated treatment effect is produced? An advantage of seeing me-
1
In this regard we concur with recent arguments of Kraemer et al.
(2002) who have the strong preference to confine discussion of mediation
(and moderation) to experimental situations in which units are randomly
assigned to the manipulated levels of the treatment variable.
2
Again, we are consistent with Kraemer et al., (2002) in making these
assumptions.
3
In fact, the models we explicate do not depend on this centering
assumption. The relevant variables could be deviated from values other
than their means, with appropriate modifications in the interpretation of
parameter estimates associated with lower-order predictor variables in
models involving higher-order interactions. See Aiken and West (1991)
and Judd and McClelland (1989).
4
Throughout we will use ’s to refer to unknown population parameters
and b’s to refer to their sample estimates. In all equations we omit the i
subscript from all variables.
5
Here and everywhere in the text, we use “overall effect” to refer to the
direct plus the indirect effect of the independent variable on the outcome.
Correspondingly, we use “residual direct effect” to refer to the direct effect
of the independent variable on the outcome controlling for the mediator.
853
MEDIATED MODERATION AND MODERATED MEDIATION
diation as a series of analytic steps (as outlined above) is that it
permits one to recognize that there is more than one way by which
an overall moderated treatment effect might be produced. We
discuss these alternatives in the later section where we outline the
analytic approach to mediated moderation.
Moderated mediation happens if the mediating process that is
responsible for producing the effect of the treatment on the out-
come depends on the value of a moderator variable. In other
words, if the moderator is an individual difference variable, then it
would mean that the mediating process that intervenes between the
treatment and the outcome is different for people who differ on
that individual difference. If the moderator is a contextual variable,
then it would mean that the mediating process varies as a function
of context. Note that this definition importantly implies mediation
(at least for some people or in some contexts), as we defined it
previously, but it does not imply any overall moderation of the
treatment effect. And in fact, we will see that it is most convenient
to clearly define moderated mediation in the prototypic case where
there is no moderation of the X to Y effect. What varies as a
function of the moderator is not the magnitude of the overall
treatment effect on the outcome but the mediating process that
produces it. Again, moderated mediation can happen in a number
of different ways. We describe these in the section below where we
discuss analytic models.
We now turn to illustrations of both mediated moderation and
moderated mediation. These examples were chosen from existing
social psychological research but they were constructed in such a
way to illustrate both processes in prototypic cases. As such, they
are reasonable models that might be examined in social psycho-
logical research, but they are deliberate oversimplifications.
To illustrate mediated moderation, consider recent work by
Smeesters, Warlop, Van Avermaet, Corneille and Yzerbyt (2003)
on the role of “morality” versus “might” primes on cooperative
versus competitive behavior in a prisoner’s dilemma choice sce-
nario. They argued that participant’s social value orientation (pro-
self vs. prosocial) would moderate the impact of such primes.
Specifically, they showed that for participants who were more
prosocial, the primes affected the choice of cooperative versus
competitive behaviors (“morality” primes increased cooperation,
compared to “might” primes) whereas for more proself partici-
pants, this difference was not found. Their explanation for the
mediating process underlying this overall moderation was that the
primes produce expectations about how the partner in the prison-
er’s dilemma game would behave. The “morality” prime induced
expectations that the partner would cooperate; the “might” prime
induced expectations that the partner would compete. And the
researchers reasoned that these primed expectations would then be
acted upon differently by prosocial and proself participants. Those
who are more prosocial should attempt to match their behavior to
what they expect from the partner: if they expect competition, their
choice should be competitive; if they expect cooperation, their
choice should be cooperative. But among more proself partici-
pants, competitive choices should predominate regardless of the
expectations: if they expect competition, their choice should be
competition; and if they expect cooperation, they should also
compete in an attempt to exploit their partner’s cooperative choice.
In sum, the researchers anticipated that the effect of the prime on
behavioral choice would depend on whether participants were
prosocial or proself (overall moderation of the treatment effect).
And they further predicted that this moderation would be mediated
by expectations about the partner’s behavior. Primes would induce
expectations about partner’s behavior, that they would either co-
operate or compete, and participants who were prosocial would
match their own behavior to their partner’s whereas those who
were more proself would compete regardless of their expectation.
To illustrate moderated mediation, we draw on research by
Petty, Schumann, Richman, and Strathman (1993) that examined
the role of positive mood in persuasion. They manipulated partic-
ipants’ mood (either positive mood or no mood induction) and
subsequently exposed them to counterattitudinal persuasive infor-
mation. They predicted that those in a positive mood would show
more persuasion than those in the control condition. And overall
they found this treatment effect. But they further were interested in
understanding differences in what mediates this overall treatment
effect as a function of a “need for cognition” individual difference
variable. They argued that for those high in need for cognition, the
mediating process producing more persuasion as a result of a
positive mood would be one where the mood causes people to
generate more positively valenced thoughts in response to the
persuasive communication and then these thoughts in turn produce
greater persuasion. on the other hand, they argued that for those
participants who were low in need for cognition, the mediating
process would not be through positively valenced thoughts. In
other words, positive thoughts would mediate the mood—persua-
sion effect for those high in need for cognition, while for those low
in need for cognition the same mood—persuasion effect would not
be mediated by positive thoughts (or would be mediated by them
less). This illustrates moderated mediation: a treatment effect is
mediated differently as a function of some moderator variable.
Need for cognition moderates the way in which the mood—persua-
sion effect is produced. Note that there is no overall moderation here:
it is not the case that the mood—persuasion effect is larger or smaller
for those who differ in need for cognition. Rather the mediating
process for the same effect was hypothesized to be different.
6
Analytic Models for Moderated Mediation and Mediated
Moderation
There are three fundamental models that underlie both mediated
moderation and moderated mediation.
7
We have already given the
6
Petty et al. (1993) note that had there been an overall moderation of the
mood effect by need for cognition, the assessment of moderated mediation
would have been problematic. Finding a pattern by which, for instance, the
effect of mood was absent for those high in need for cognition simply
“precludes examination of any differential mediation of the positive mood
effect for people high and low in NC because there was no effect of
positive mood to mediate for the high-NC individuals.” (p. 9) Although not
intractable, we would agree that interpretational problems arise in talking
about moderated mediation whenever the magnitude of the treatment effect
on the outcome variable varies as a function of the moderator.
7
The models that we give here appear somewhat different from those
proposed by Baron and Kenny (1986, p. 1179) in their brief treatment of
mediated moderation and moderated mediation. This is because in their
example the moderator was a manipulated independent variable and it was
then subsequently measured (as a manipulation check) to serve as a
mediator. Our approach, utilizing conceptually different moderator and
mediator variables and a single set of equations, is more parsimonious.
854
MULLER, JUDD, AND YZERBYT
first of these as Equation 4, to assess moderation of the overall
treatment effect:
Y
40
41
X
42
Mo
43
XMo
4
(4)
This model allows the overall treatment effect of Equation 1 to
be moderated by Mo. The second model allows the treatment effect
on the mediator, in Equation 2, to be moderated:
Me
50
51
X
52
Mo
53
XMo
5
(5)
And the third model is a moderated version of Equation 3, in
which both the mediator’s (partial) effect on the outcome and the
residual effect of the treatment on the outcome, controlling for the
mediator, are allowed to be moderated:
Y
60
61
X
62
Mo
63
XMo
64
Me
65
MeMo
6
(6)
In all three models, we are making the same assumptions about
all variables that we did earlier. Namely, there are two levels of X
(contrast coded) to which experimental units have been randomly
assigned, X and Mo are therefore uncorrelated, and both Me and
Mo have been centered.
8
Given these assumptions, the slope
parameters in these three models can be interpreted as defined in
Table 1.
In Figure 1, we present the fundamental model that subsumes
both mediated moderation and moderated mediation. The top panel
in this Figure represents the overall treatment effect on the out-
come, and the extent to which it is moderated. Accordingly, the
overall treatment effect depends on the level of the moderator, to
the extent that
43
departs from zero. The bottom panel presents
the mediated model, allowing the treatment effect on the mediator
to be moderated (to the extent that
53
departs from zero), the
mediator’s partial effect on the outcome to be moderated (to the
extent that
65
departs from zero), and the residual direct effect of
the treatment on the outcome (controlling for the mediator) to be
moderated (to the extent that
63
departs from zero).
From this Figure, the overall (moderated) treatment effect is
41
43
Mo. Using the estimated values for these parameters,
one can use this expression to estimate “simple” overall treatment
effects at particular levels of Mo. The (moderated) indirect effect,
via the mediator, equals
51
53
Mo
64
65
Mo . And the
residual (moderated) treatment effect equals
61
63
Mo.
Again, the parameter estimates from these expressions can be used
to estimate “simple” effects at particular levels of Mo.
Parallel to what we saw in the case of simple mediation (Equa-
tions 1–3), there exists a fundamental equality among the param-
eters of these models, focusing on the moderation of the indirect
and residual direct effects. In the Appendix, we prove that at the
level of the population parameters, making the assumptions we
have made, the following equality holds:
43
63
64
53
65
51
(7)
This equality will not exactly hold in terms of parameter esti-
mates, from a sample of data, unless the moderator is dichotomous
and contrast coded (see Appendix). Nevertheless, this equality
provides us with fundamental insights into the definitions of me-
diated moderation and moderated mediation.
We now turn to these definitions and what they imply about the
three models we have just identified. Doing this provides us with
analytic strategies for estimating both mediated moderation and
moderated mediation. And the consideration of these processes, in
the context of these models, will provide insights about the dif-
ferent ways in which each process might be produced.
Mediated Moderation
The above Models 4 through 6 are used to establish mediated
moderation. Equation 7 establishes an equality condition on the
8
When the parameters of these models are estimated in a sample of data,
additional assumptions are necessary, of course, to derive their standard
errors. Namely, residuals must be independent, normally distributed, and
have a common variance.
Table 1
Interpretation of the Slope Parameters in Equations 4, 5, and 6
Slope
parameters
Interpretation of slope parameters
41
Overall treatment effect on Y at the average level of Mo
42
Moderator effect on Y on average across the two treatment
levels
43
Change in overall treatment effect on Y as Mo increases
51
Treatment effect on Me at the average level of Mo
52
Moderator effect on Me on average across the two
treatment levels
53
Change in treatment effect on Me as Mo increases
61
Residual direct treatment effect on Y at the average level
of Mo
62
Moderator effect on Y on average within the two treatment
levels and at the average level of Me
63
Change in residual direct treatment effect on Y as Mo
increases
64
Mediator effect on Y on average within the two treatment
levels and at the average level of Mo
65
Change in mediator effect on Y as Mo increases
Figure 1. Models illustrating moderated mediation and mediated
moderation.
855
MEDIATED MODERATION AND MODERATED MEDIATION
parameters from these models. one could first focus on the two
parameters on the left side of this equality, that is,
43
and
63
.
With mediated moderation, there is overall moderation of the
treatment effect, that is,
43
0, and the question then is whether
the mediating process accounts for this moderation. If it does, then
the moderation of the residual direct effect of the treatment should
be reduced compared to the moderation of the overall treatment
effect, that is,
63
should be smaller in absolute value than
43
. For
this to be the case, we see on the right side of Equation 7 that there
must be mediation and one or both of the indirect paths from the
treatment to the outcome must be moderated. That is, either the
effect of X on Me depends on the moderator (
53
0, and the
average partial effect of Me on Y [
64
] is nonzero) and/or the
partial effect of Me on Y depends on the moderator (
65
0, and
the average effect of X on Me [
51
] is nonzero).
In light of this, to demonstrate mediated moderation in a sample
of data, one estimates Models 4 through 6. In Model 4, we would
expect b
43
to be significant, indicating overall treatment modera-
tion. In Models 5 and 6, either (or both) of two patterns should
exist: both b
53
and b
64
are significant or both b
51
and b
65
are
significant. And, as a result, the moderation of the residual treat-
ment effect, b
63
, should be reduced in magnitude (and may be
nonsignificant in the case of what might be called “full” mediated
moderation) compared to the moderation of the overall treatment
effect.
Moderated Mediation
In the prototypic case of moderated mediation, there is an
overall treatment effect (
41
) and the magnitude of this effect does
not depend on the moderator (
43
0). However, the potency of
the mediating process depends on the moderator. Accordingly,
either the effect of the treatment on the mediator depends on the
moderator (
53
0) or the partial effect of the mediator on the
outcome depends on the moderator (
65
0), or both. Parallel to
this, if the treatment effect on the mediator depends on the mod-
erator (
53
0), then there must be a partial effect of the mediator
on the outcome on average (
64
0), or if the partial effect of the
mediator on the outcome depends on the moderator (
65
0), then
there should be an overall treatment effect on the mediator
(
51
0). In other words, at least one of the products on the
right-hand side of the equality in Equation 7 must depart from
zero.
Accordingly, if the right-hand side of the equality in Equation 7
departs from zero, then too must the left-hand side parameter
difference. We have assumed that the first term in this difference
equals zero (i.e.,
43
0, there is no overall moderation of the
treatment effect). This implies that the second term (
63
) differs
from zero.
In sum, moderated mediation implies that the indirect effect
between the treatment and the outcome depends on the moderator.
That is, either the effect from X to Me depends on the moderator
(
53
0, and the average partial effect of Me on Y [
64
] is
nonzero) and/or the partial effect of Me on Y depends on the
moderator (
65
0, and the average effect of X on Me [
51
] is
nonzero). In either case, given no overall moderation of the treat-
ment effect (i.e.,
43
0), this implies that the residual direct
treatment effect on the outcome, controlling for the mediator, is
moderated (i.e.,
63
0).
9
In light of this, to demonstrate moderated mediation in a sample
of data, one estimates Models 4 through 6. In Model 4, the
prototypic case leads to the expectation that b
41
is significantly
different from zero, while b
43
is not. In Models 5 and 6, either (or
both) of two patterns should exist: both b
53
and b
64
are significant
or both b
51
and b
65
are significant. A consequence is that the
residual treatment effect should now be moderated, that is b
63
may
be significant. Although this is likely, we do not believe that the
significance of b
63
should be seen as a necessary condition for
establishing moderated mediation.
Summary
In sum, for mediated moderation, there is overall moderation,
produced by the mediating process, and when this process is
controlled, the residual moderation of the treatment effect is re-
duced. What we have called prototypic moderated mediation is
found when there is an unmoderated overall treatment effect, but
the indirect effect of the treatment via the mediator is moderated.
As a result, the residual treatment effect is likely to be moderated.
Data-Based Examples
In this section, we return to the two examples we used earlier to
illustrate mediated moderation and moderated mediation, but this
time with hypothetical data
10
and illustrative model estimates from
those data.
Mediated Moderation Example
The example that we used to illustrate the definition of mediated
moderation focused on cooperative behavior in a prisoner’s di-
lemma situation (Smeesters et al., 2003). Participants were primed
with either “might” or “morality” primes and then engaged in a
one-trial prisoner’s dilemma with a fictitious partner. Additionally,
participants’ social value orientation (from proself to prosocial)
was measured. The researchers found an overall moderation of the
effect of the prime by social value orientation: those participants
who were more prosocial indicated they would more likely coop-
erate under the “morality” prime while they were more likely to
compete under the “might” prime. on the other hand, more pro-
self participants tended to choose competition regardless of the
prime. It was then suggested that this moderation was produced by
expectations about the partner. More specifically, the “morality”
prime led to expectations that the partner would cooperate, while
the “might” prime led to expectations of partner competition. And
participants proself versus prosocial status was found to moderate
the impact of these partner expectations on behavior: Prosocial
participants attempted to match the behaviors expected from their
9
In rare cases, it is possible that the effect of the treatment on the
mediator is moderated in one direction while the effect of the mediator on
the outcome is moderated in the opposite direction. In these cases, the
overall moderation of the treatment effect may be the same as the moder-
ation of the residual direct treatment effect.
10
We used simulated data instead of real data to keep things parsimo-
nious. Additionally, they are publicly available, as indicated below, so that
the reader can recreate our analyses.
856
MULLER, JUDD, AND YZERBYT
partner, while proself participants competed regardless of how
they expected their partner to behave.
For this illustration, data were generated for 100 cases on four
variables:
11
(a) A dichotomous treatment variable indicating the type of
prime received (X: referred to as PRIME): “might” priming ( 1)
versus “morality” priming ( 1). Values on this variable were
randomly assigned to the 100 cases.
(b) A continuous moderator variable (MO: referred to as SVO
for social value orientation, with lower numbers for more proself
participants and higher numbers for those who are more prosocial):
randomly generated from a normal distribution with a mean of
zero and a standard deviation of 1.35. This variable was centered
in the sample. Sample values ranged from 3.369 to
3.833.
(c) A continuous mediator variable (ME: referred to as EXP for
expectations about the partner’s behavior, with higher numbers
indicating that the partner was expected to be more cooperative).
It was generated to be a function of prime, adding a random error
component. Again, it was centered in the sample and had a
standard deviation of 7.913.
(d) A continuous outcome variable (Y: referred to as BEH for
behavior, with higher numbers indicating a greater probability of
cooperative behavioral choices). This outcome variable was con-
structed to be affected by the prime, social value orientation,
expectations, and the Mediator
Moderator interaction, plus a
random error component.
As just described, these data were generated to be consistent
with the mediated moderation hypothesis, that is, that the overall
effect of the treatment (PRIME) on the outcome (BEH) would be
moderated by social value orientation (SVO) and that this inter-
action would be due to the effect of the treatment on the mediator
(EXP) and the moderation of the mediator effect on the outcome
(BEH) by social value orientation (SVO).
Table 2 contains the univariate statistics and bivariate correla-
tions for all four variables. Table 3 presents the regression models
that estimate Equations 4 through 6 with these variables. Presented
here are the unstandardized coefficients (b) and their associated t
statistics. In these equations, there are product predictors included
to estimate the interactions (PRIMESVO
PRIME*SVO;
EXPSVO
EXP*SVO).
The results from Equation 4 find evidence for the predicted
interaction between PRIME and SVO on the outcome variable,
BEH. As predicted, “morality” primes are associated with a higher
probability of cooperative behaviors than “might” primes, and this
difference increases as participants are more prosocial and less
proself. The treatment variable’s (PRIME) effect on the outcome
(BEH) is moderated by social orientation (SVO).
The results from Equation 5 show an effect of the prime on the
mediator, that is expectations about the partner’s behavior (EXP).
Moreover, this effect is not moderated by social orientation (the
PRIMESVO coefficient is not significant). In order to help with
the interpretation of the indirect effect in these data it is informa-
tive to calculate the simple effects of the prime (PRIME) on the
mediator (EXP) at different levels of the moderator (SVO), as
discussed earlier. Because the moderator varies continuously, we
have calculated these simple effects at values of one standard
deviation (1.398) above and below the mean SVO score:
For High SVO ( 1 SD): b
51
b
53
Mo
2.692
0.089 1.398
2.816
For Low SVO ( 1 SD): b
51
b
53
Mo
2.692
0.089( 1.398)
2.568
The estimation of Equation 6 reveals a significant effect of the
mediator (EXP) by moderator (SVO) interaction on the outcome
(the EXPSVO coefficient is significant). We can again calculate
the simple effect of EXP on BEH at values of SVO one standard
deviation above and below the mean SVO score:
For High SVO ( 1 SD): b
64
b
65
Mo
0.840
0.765 1.398
1.909
For Low SVO
1 SD : b
64
b
65
Mo
0.840
0.765
1.398
0.229
Taking the product of the two simple effects for each of the two
values of SVO, we get the total indirect effects through the
mediator (EXP) for each of these two values:
For High SVO ( 1 SD): 2.816*1.909
5.377
For Low SVO ( 1 SD): 2.568*
0.229
0.589
Accordingly, for all participants, regardless of social orientation,
the cooperative prime increases expectations that the partner will
cooperate. And such expectations lead to cooperative behavior on
the part of the participants but only in the case of those who are
high (prosocial) on social value orientation. It would seem that the
moderation of the overall effect of the prime (by social value
orientation) can be understood from the fact that the cooperative
prime induces cooperative expectations among all participants, but
those expectations translate into cooperative behavior only for
those who have a cooperative social value orientation.
Consistent with this, Equation 6 also reveals that the residual
direct effect of the prime on the outcome (BEH) is less (and
actually not at all in this example) moderated by social value
orientation, once the mediator (EXP) and its interaction with SVO
11
The raw data are available at http://www.psp.ucl.ac.be/mediation/
Table 2
Univariate and Bivariate Statistics for Mediated Moderation
Example
Variable
PRIME
(Treatment)
SVO
(Moderator)
EXP
(Mediator)
BEH
(Outcome)
M
.000
.000
.000
58.171
SD
1.005
1.398
7.913
14.691
Correlations
PRIME
.004
.342**
.314**
SVO
.016
.165
EXP
.385
BEH
Note. SVO
social value orientation; EXP
expectations about part-
ner’s behavior; BEH
behavior.
** p
.01.
857
MEDIATED MODERATION AND MODERATED MEDIATION
are controlled. Indeed, the coefficient associated with the PRIME
* SVO interaction has been reduced from 2.508 (in Equation 4) to
0.041 (in Equation 6). once we control for the mediator and allow
the indirect effect via the mediator to be moderated, the residual
direct effect of the prime on the outcome no longer depends on this
moderator.
As we made clear earlier, mediated moderation may be pro-
duced in either (or both) of two ways. In the first way, the
moderator affects the magnitude of the treatment effect on the
mediator (and this is found in conjunction with a mediator effect in
Equation 6). Alternatively, and this is the situation illustrated in
our example, the moderator affects the magnitude of the media-
tor’s partial effect on the outcome (and this is found in conjunction
with a treatment effect on the mediator in Equation 5).
Moderated Mediation Example
We illustrate this analysis with data based loosely on the mod-
erated mediation example we highlighted in the introduction (Petty
et al., 1993). Recall that in this example the effect of positive mood
(vs. control) on persuasion was thought to be more mediated by
positive valenced thoughts in the case of individuals who are high
in “need for cognition” than in the case of individuals low in “need
for cognition.” For this illustration data were generated for 100
cases on four variables:
12
(a) A dichotomous treatment variable (X: referred to as MOOD):
positive mood induction ( 1) versus no mood induction ( 1).
Values on this variable were randomly assigned to the 100 cases.
(b) A continuous moderator variable (MO: referred to as NFC
for need for cognition scores with higher numbers for higher need
for cognition): randomly generated from a normal distribution with
a mean of zero and a standard deviation of 1.35. This variable was
centered in the sample. Sample values ranged from 4.82 to
3.07.
(c) A continuous mediator variable (ME: referred to as POS for
positive valenced thoughts, with higher numbers indicating more
positive valenced thoughts). This is the mediator. It was generated
to be a function of the treatment variable and its interaction with
NFC, adding in a random error component. Again it was centered
in the sample and ranged from 18.05 to
21.74.
(d) A continuous outcome variable (Y: referred to as ATT for
attitude change, with higher numbers indicating more attitude
change). This outcome variable was constructed to be affected by
the treatment variable and the mediator, plus a random error
component.
As just described, these data were constructed following the
theoretical model of Petty et al. (1993), so that there was an overall
treatment effect on attitude change, unmoderated by need for
cognition. For those high in need for cognition, the treatment
variable affected the mediator (positive valenced thoughts) more
than for those low in need for cognition, while this mediator
affected the outcome equally for all participants. Unlike Petty et al.
(1993), we assumed that need for cognition scores were continuously
measured rather than dichotomizing the variable at its median.
Table 4 contains univariate statistics and bivariate correlations
for all four variables. Table 5 presents the regression models that
estimate Equations 4 through 6 with these variables. Presented
here are the unstandardized coefficients (b) and their associated t
statistics. In these equations, product predictors are included to
estimate the interactions (MOODNFC
MOOD*NFC; POSNFC
POS*NFC).
The results from Equation 4 indicate an overall effect of the
treatment, MOOD, on the outcome variable, ATT. This effect is
not moderated by need for cognition, NFC.
In Equation 5, the mediator, POS, is the criterion. Here, there is
a significant effect of MOOD and a significant MOOD
NFC
interaction. This significant interaction is indicative of moderated
mediation, in that it means that the magnitude of the indirect effect
of MOOD, via the mediator, varies in magnitude as a function of
NFC. As in the previous example, it is useful to again calculate the
simple effects of MOOD on POS at one standard deviation (1.405)
above and below the NFC mean:
For High NFC ( 1 SD): b
51
b
53
Mo
4.336
1.256 1.405
6.101
12
The raw data are available at http://www.psp.ucl.ac.be/mediation/
Table 3
Least Squares Regression Results for Mediated Moderation Example
Equation 4
(criterion BEH)
Equation 5
(criterion EXP)
Equation 6
(criterion BEH)
Predictors
b
t
b
t
b
t
X: PRIME
4.580
3.40**
2.692
3.57**
2.169
2.03*
(b
41
)
(b
51
)
(b
61
)
MO: SVO
2.042
2.09*
0.085
0.16
2.569
3.54
(b
42
)
(b
52
)
(b
62
)
XMO: PRIMESVO
2.574
2.64**
0.089
0.16
0.041
0.05
(b
43
)
(b
53
)
(b
63
)
ME: EXP
0.840
6.05**
(b
64
)
MEMO: EXPSVO
0.765
7.91**
(b
65
)
BEH
behavior; EXP
expectations about partner’s behavior; MO
moderator variable; SVO
social value
orientation; ME
mediator variable.
* p
.05. ** p
.01.
858
MULLER, JUDD, AND YZERBYT
For Low NFC ( 1 SD): b
51
b
53
Mo
4.336
1.256( 1.405)
2.571
For someone well above the mean on need for cognition, pos-
itive mood, compared to control, leads to a considerably higher
POS score. on the other hand, for someone well below the mean
on the moderator, there is less of an effect of MOOD on POS.
From the Equation 6 results, importantly we see that there is a
significant effect of the mediator (POS) on the outcome. More-
over, this is not moderated by need for cognition (the POSNFC
coefficient is not significant). In order to help interpretations we
can however, again calculate the simple effects of the mediator on
the outcome at values of NFC one standard deviation above and
below the mean:
For High NFC ( 1 SD): b
64
b
65
Mo
1.248
.036 1.405
1.197
For Low NFC ( 1 SD): b
64
b
65
Mo
1.248
.036( 1.405)
1.299
Taking the product of the two simple effects, once at one
standard deviation above the mean NFC and once at one standard
deviation below the mean, we get the total indirect effects at the
two values:
For High NFC ( 1 SD): 6.101*1.197
7.303
For Low NFC ( 1 SD): 2.571*1.299
3.340
Equation 6 also reveals, as might be expected, that the residual
direct effect of the treatment (MOOD) on the outcome is moder-
ated, once the mediator is controlled. That is, there is a significant
effect of the MOOD
NFC interaction. Since the overall effect of
the treatment on the outcome does not vary as a function of NFC
(from Equation 4), and since the indirect effect via the mediator
does vary as a function of NFC, then it is the case that the residual
direct effect, controlling for the mediator, is moderated by NFC.
Again, we can calculate the two simple residual treatment effects
at the two levels of NFC (plus and minus one standard deviation
from the mean):
For High NFC ( 1 SD): 1.480
2.169 1.405
1.567
For Low NFC ( 1 SD): 1.480
2.169( 1.405)
4.527
These results as a whole make very clear that the indirect effect,
via the mediator, is much higher when NFC is high rather than
low, while the residual direct effect is much higher when NFC is
low rather than high. This pattern is what is expected under
prototypical moderated mediation.
This example illustrates a case where there is moderated medi-
ation because the effect of the treatment on the mediator depends
on the moderator. As a result, the overall magnitude of the indirect
effect via the mediator depends on the moderator. Alternatively, as
we made clear previously, moderated mediation could also happen
when the effect of the mediator on the outcome depends on the
moderator, producing a moderated indirect effect in a different
manner.
Table 4
Univariate and Bivariate Statistics for Moderated Mediation
Example
Variable
MOOD
(Treatment)
NFC
(Moderator)
POS
(Mediator)
ATT
(Outcome)
M
.000
.000
.000
1.98
SD
1.005
1.405
8.322
16.79
Correlations
MOOD
.023
.521**
.405**
NFC
.068
.110
POS
.629**
ATT
Note. NFC
need for cognition; POS
positive valenced thoughts;
ATT
attitude change.
* p
.05. ** p
.01.
Table 5
Least Squares Regression Results for Moderated Mediation Example
Equation 4
(Criterion ATT)
Equation 5
(Criterion POS)
Equation 6
(Criterion ATT)
Predictors
b
t
b
t
b
t
X: MOOD
6.813
4.415**
4.336
6.219**
1.480
.957
(b
41
)
(b
51
)
(b
61
)
MO: NFC
1.268
1.117
.767
1.496
.356
.366
(b
42
)
(b
52
)
(b
62
)
XMO: MOODNFC
.691
.609
1.256
2.450*
2.169
2.112*
(b
43
)
(b
53
)
(b
63
)
ME: POS
1.248
6.613**
(b
64
)
MEMO: POSNFC
.036
.279
(b
65
)
Note. ATT
attitude change; POS
positive valenced thoughts; MO
moderator variable; NFC
need for
cognition; ME
mediator variable.
* p
.05. ** p
.01.
859
MEDIATED MODERATION AND MODERATED MEDIATION
Integrating the Two Processes
Although our purpose in this article has been to clearly define
both mediated moderation and moderated mediation, it should by
now be apparent that ultimately they both rest on the same analytic
models and the same fundamental equality, given in Equation 7:
43
63
64
53
65
51
(7)
We can conceptualize a continuum of analytic cases, varying in
the relative magnitude of the difference on the left side of this
equality (and thereby varying as well as a function of the terms on
its right side). The continuum is defined by the relative magnitude
of the difference between the absolute values of both
43
and
63
:
|
43
|
|
63
|.
13
In the middle of this continuum lies the situation in
which this difference equals zero. In this case, neither mediated
moderation nor moderated mediation can be said to occur. Below
this point lies the range of cases in which |
43
|
|
63
|
0 and
above it lies the range of cases in which |
43
|
|
63
|
0.
Our consistent definition of mediated moderation rests on this
difference being greater than zero: There is moderation of the
overall treatment effect in Equation 4 (
43
), and this is reduced in
magnitude once the moderation of the indirect effect is controlled
(
63
).
One might then be tempted to say that the other process,
moderated mediation, occurs whenever the difference is smaller
than zero, that is, whenever it is the case that the moderation of the
residual treatment effect (
63
) is greater than the moderation of the
overall treatment effect (
43
). But we have been very careful
throughout our discussion to always talk about moderated media-
tion in the “prototypic” case when there is no overall moderation,
that is, when
43
0. Our reasons for this insistence on defining
the “prototypic” case of moderated mediation can now be made
clear. If one defines moderated mediation without the assumption
that
43
0, then we would suggest that any point along the
continuum we have just defined (other than zero), including points
where |
43
|
|
63
|
0, can be seen as moderated mediation, de-
pending on the theoretical intentions of the researcher. That is, if
moderated mediation consists solely in identifying whether a me-
diating process between the treatment and the outcome is moder-
ated, then any time the terms on the right or left side of Equation
7 depart from zero, this can be said to occur.
This continuum of values for |
43
|
|
63
| thus provides concep-
tual insights about the processes we are examining. When this
difference equals zero, then neither mediated moderation nor mod-
erated mediation can be said to occur. When mediated moderation
is hypothesized this difference must be positive. on the other hand,
when moderated mediation is hypothesized this difference will be
negative if
43
0, which is the case for what we have called
prototypic moderated mediation. If one defines moderated medi-
ation as occurring whenever the mediating indirect effect is mod-
erated, relaxing the restriction of the prototypic case that
43
0, then moderated mediation can be said to occur regardless of
the sign of |
43
|
|
63
|, so long as it is not zero. Accordingly,
under this more relaxed definition, every case of mediated mod-
eration could be called moderated mediation, but the reverse is not
true.
In light of this conclusion, it is important to emphasize that the
relative magnitude of the |
43
|
|
63
| difference does not by itself
determine whether one is examining a case of mediated modera-
tion or moderated mediation. Other issues are involved. For in-
stance, as we have made clear, an overall treatment moderation is
assumed in the case of mediated moderation (i.e.,
43
0).
Ultimately, we would suggest that whether reference is made to
mediated moderation or moderated mediation depends on the
theoretical goals of the researcher, having examined the relevant
models. If there is overall moderation of the treatment effect and
if the models are examined in order to determine what is the
process responsible for this overall moderation, then the analysis is
in the service of mediated moderation goals. If, on the other hand,
one suspects that the process mediating a treatment effect depends
on a particular moderator, then the analysis is in the service of
moderated mediation goals. And this may be the case regardless of
whether there is or is not moderation of the overall treatment
effect. In other words, while we have defined the prototypic
moderated mediation case as occurring when the overall treatment
effect is unmoderated, ultimately moderated mediation is the goal
whenever the analysis is undertaken to understand how a mediat-
ing process is moderated, regardless of whether the overall treat-
ment effect is itself moderated.
Remaining Issues and Conclusion
Overall Significance Testing
Our focus throughout has been on specifying the conditions
necessary to establish mediated moderation and moderated medi-
ation. To do this, we have tended to focus on the values of
population parameters and only occasionally have we discussed
their estimates, their standard errors, and traditional significance
testing. Following the advice we have given on these matters, our
approach to testing for mediated moderation and moderated me-
diation may seem rather piecemeal. For instance, in the case of
both processes, we argue that at least one of the two indirect effects
(from the treatment through the mediator to the outcome) should
be significantly moderated, while the other indirect effect should
be significant on average. Additionally, in demonstrating mediated
moderation, we argued that overall moderation should be signifi-
cant. In outlining what we have called prototypic moderated me-
diation, we suggested that the overall treatment effect (i.e., b
41
)
should be significant, overall moderation should not be found (i.e.,
b
43
, ns), but the residual effect of the treatment on the outcome may
be significant (i.e., b
63
).
One might be tempted to argue that some overall test of both
processes might be more appropriate. If so, then the obvious
candidate here is to provide tests of whether the estimated terms on
either side of the equality of Equation 7 differ significantly from
zero. Thus one might be tempted to develop tests of either of the
following null hypotheses and call these overall tests of mediated
moderation or moderated mediation:
Ho:
43
63
0.
Ho:
64
53
65
51
0.
This would provide overall tests analogous to those provided by
MacKinnon et al. (2002) in the case of simple mediation. It would
13
We are assuming here that both of these parameters have the same
sign.
860
MULLER, JUDD, AND YZERBYT
certainly seem that some of the tests examined by MacKinnon et
al. could be extended to testing at least the first of the above null
hypotheses.
Although such a test might be a desirable addition to our
piecemeal approach, we think that such an overall test would not
be sufficient in and of itself to demonstrate either mediated mod-
eration or moderated mediation. Just as we believe that establish-
ing simple mediation requires that the researcher examine a series
of models and plausible inferences, so too uncovering the more
complex processes we are dealing with involves a bit of detective
work, examining whether the overall pattern of results, including
confidence intervals for a number of different parameters in the
models, support the notions of mediated moderation and moder-
ated mediation. We also believe that it is of theoretical interest for
researchers to differentiate the alternative ways in which mediated
moderation and moderated mediation may be produced, and this
would not occur if a single overall test was the only thing
examined.
Tests of Simple Mediation
In the section where we presented analyses of hypothetical data,
we calculated simple effects to help interpretation. one may want
to actually test these simple effects and, by doing so, to test simple
mediation at various levels of a moderator. In order to do so, one
would need to deviate the moderator not from its mean (as in
centering), but from whatever values are of interest. So for in-
stance, with a dichotomous moderator, one may want to test simple
mediation at each level of the moderator. Here, instead of using a
contrast-coded moderator, one would use codes that give a value of
zero to first one, and then the other, of the two levels of the
moderator (see Judd & McClelland, 1989, for tests of simple
effects in the context of multiple regressions). This would be
equivalent to the use of dummy coding conventions. Consider first
Equations 5 and 6 when we use Mo
a
to code the moderator, with
values of 0 for level A and
1 for level B. (The superscript
indicates the level of the moderator receiving the 0 value). The
resulting equations are:
Me
50
a
51
a
X
52
a
Mo
a
53
a
XMo
a
5
(5a)
Y
60
a
61
a
X
62
a
Mo
a
63
a
XMo
a
64
a
Me
65
a
MeMo
a
6
(6a)
In these equations,
51
a
now equals the simple effect of the
treatment on the mediator for individuals in level A of the Mod-
erator. Likewise,
64
a
equals the simple effect of the mediator on
the outcome for level A. And
61
a
equals the simple residual effect
of the treatment on the outcome, over and above mediation, again
for level A of the moderator. The significance tests associated with
the estimates of these coefficients can be used to test whether those
simple effects differ reliably from zero.
Alternatively we can redefine the moderator as Mo
b
(i.e., Level
B
0; Level A
1):
Me
50
b
51
b
X
52
b
Mo
b
53
b
XMo
b
5
(5b)
Y
60
b
61
b
X
62
b
Mo
b
63
b
XMo
b
64
b
Me
65
b
MeMo
b
6
(6b)
And now
51
b
,
64
b
, and
61
b
represent the simple effects for level
B of the moderator and one can test whether their estimates differ
from zero reliably. In the more general case of a continuous
moderator, one would use the same strategy but would deviate the
moderator from values of interest, for instance one standard devi-
ation above and below its mean.
Although these alternative specifications of the moderator allow
one to estimate and test the simple mediation effects at different
levels of the moderator, it has to be remembered that to demon-
strate moderated mediation it is necessary to show that these
simple mediation effects significantly depend on the moderator. In
other words, it is not sufficient simply to show significant simple
mediation at one level of the moderator but not at another. one
must show that the process of mediation is significantly different
at different levels of the moderator and this is done by testing the
various interaction terms in Equations 5 and 6, as previously
described.
Extensions to Purely Correlational Data
Throughout our presentation we have made the strong assump-
tion that the treatment is an experimentally manipulated one,
permitting causal inference about the overall treatment effect and
its effect on the mediator. This has also permitted us to assume that
the treatment is independent of the moderator, an important con-
dition necessary for the derivation we gave in the Appendix for the
equality underlying both mediated moderation and moderated me-
diation (Equation 7).
In the literature on mediation, we have seen (all too often for our
tastes) researchers take three measured variables, claim a media-
tional model, and believe, because a “test of mediation” (e.g., one
of those recommended by MacKinnon et al., 2002) comes out, that
they have established mediation. But in this case, there is no way
to decide which variable is the treatment whose effect is mediated,
which is the mediator, and which is the outcome. It is not our goal
to encourage these kinds of practices, even though we recognize
that some will use the models that we develop in this article to
claim either mediated moderation or moderated mediation in the
presence of a purely correlational research design. If the research-
ers’ causal model is the correct one, then it stands to reason that the
models we have outlined in this article can in fact be used to
examine issues of mediated moderation and moderated mediation
with purely correlational data. Although the fundamental relation-
ship of Equation 7 would not be expected to hold, the resulting
parameter estimates would be interpretable in a manner consistent
with what we have presented.
Practical Issues
A potentially important issue that we have not previously dis-
cussed concerns the biasing effects of measurement error in the
moderator, mediator, and outcome variables. Unfortunately the
direction of bias due to measurement error is difficult to know,
given the complexities of the models we are dealing with. The
obvious solution is to use multiple indicators of these variables,
weighting them appropriately, as in a structural equation latent
variable approach (SEM). With a dichotomous moderator, this
approach can be implemented by a multiple Group SEM estima-
tion (but this assumes that the moderator is measured perfectly).
861
MEDIATED MODERATION AND MODERATED MEDIATION
With a continuously measured moderator, there exists no straight-
forward solution for estimating SEM models with latent variable
interactions (see Jaccard & Wan, 1995; Kenny & Judd, 1984; Li et
al., 1998).
One obvious alternative solution is to measure all three variables
(mediator, moderator, and outcome) with multiple indicators
which are then combined or aggregated into index scores, assum-
ing they manifest high internal consistency. Ultimately, for the
sorts of interactive models that underlie the analyses we have
outlined, we suspect that this is the most efficient way to deal with
the measurement error issue. Of course, this approach deals only
with random, rather than systematic, measurement error.
Another practical issue to discuss in estimating these processes
is that of sufficient statistical power. The literature on simple
mediation suggests that some tests of mediation may be relatively
unpowerful (MacKinnon et al., 2002). This may also be true in the
cases we have been considering. The obvious advice is that the
researcher who is interested in examining moderated mediation
and mediated moderation should do whatever he or she can to
maximize statistical power.
We have confined our discussion to cases with a single mediator
and a single moderator. In the case of moderated mediation, it
seems likely that there may exist multiple indirect paths from the
treatment to the outcome, with one mediated pathway being more
potent for some individuals and another being more important for
others. In this case, with potentially collinear mediators (and
moderators), power issues obviously would become particularly
important.
Conclusion
When we first set out to write this article, our goals were to
clearly define both mediated moderation and moderated mediation
and to elaborate in greater detail than had been done previously the
analytic models underlying both. Earlier discussions of these pro-
cesses (Baron & Kenny, 1986; James & Brett, 1984; Wegener &
Fabrigar, 2000) are certainly informative, but it seemed to us that
both processes deserved a more detailed treatment. Thus, we
envisioned ourselves writing a largely didactic piece, laying out
the analytic roadmaps for these alternative analyses.
Although we hope that we have accomplished this, it became
apparent to us, as our ideas (and this article) evolved, that we had
much more to say at a theoretical level about the inherent simi-
larities between the two processes. They both rely on the same
analytic models, they both imply moderated indirect effects of the
treatment variable on the outcome, and they both imply that the
overall moderation of the treatment effect is altered once the
(moderated) mediating process is controlled. At the same time, we
would not want to claim that moderated mediation and mediated
moderation are one and the same. By defining the prototypic
moderated mediation case, we can clearly differentiate between
them. Additionally, mediated moderation clearly does imply that
the overall moderation of a treatment effect is reduced once the
(moderated) mediating process is controlled.
That said, however, we also have argued that if one allows for
the possibility of moderated mediation in cases where there is
overall moderation of the treatment effect, then the distinction
between the two processes can become more a matter of theoret-
ical preference than anything else. Is the emphasis on the reduction
of the moderation of the treatment effect once the (moderated)
mediating process is controlled? Or is the emphasis more on the
fact that there is a moderated mediating process?
The analytic models are clear cut and the two processes can be
defined in the prototypic cases. But these, as we have already said,
represent probably two sides of the same coin. In talking about that
coin, we can either concentrate on describing each side in turn, or
we can recognize that they both define the common coin.
References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
51, 1173–1182.
Jaccard, J., & Wan, C. K. (1995). Measurement error in the analysis of
interaction effects between continuous predictors using multiple regres-
sion: Multiple indicator and structural equation approaches. Psycholog-
ical Bulletin, 117, 348–357.
James, L. R., & Brett, J. M. (1984). Mediators, moderators, and tests for
mediation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 307–321.
Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1981). Process analysis: Estimating medi-
ation in treatment evaluation. Evaluation Review, 5, 602–619.
Judd, C. M., & McClelland, G. H. (1989). Data analysis: A model com-
parison approach. San Diego, CA: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (1984). Estimating the nonlinear and inter-
active effects of latent variables. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 201–210.
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social
psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The
handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 233–265). Boston,
MA: McGraw-Hill.
Kraemer, H. C., Wilson, G. T., Fairburn, C. G., & Agras, W. S. (2002).
Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical
trials. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 877–884.
Li, F., Harmer, P., Duncan, T. E., Duncan, S. C., Acock, A., & Boles, S.
(1998). Approaches to testing interaction effects using structural equa-
tion modeling methodology. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33,
1–39.
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., &
Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other
intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104.
MacKinnon, D. P., Warsi, G., & Dwyer, J. H. (1995). A simulation study
of mediated effect measures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 30,
41–62.
Petty, R. E., Schumann, D. W., Richman, S. A., & Strathman, A. J. (1993).
Positive mood and persuasion: Different roles for affect under high- and
low-elaboration conditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 64, 5–20.
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non-
experimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psycho-
logical Methods, 7, 422–445.
Smeesters, D., Warlop, L., Van Avermaet, E., Corneille, O., & Yzerbyt,
V. Y. (2003). Do not prime hawks with doves: The interplay of
construct activation and consistency of social value orientation on
cooperative behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
84, 972–987.
Wegener, D. T., & Fabrigar, L. R. (2000). Analysis and design for non-
experimental data: Addressing causal and noncausal hypothesis. In H. T.
Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and
personality psychology (pp. 412–450). New York: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
862
MULLER, JUDD, AND YZERBYT
Appendix
In the case of both mediated moderation and moderated mediation, the
analysis proceeds by estimating three models:
Y
40
41
X
42
Mo
43
XMo
4
(4)
Me
50
51
X
52
Mo
53
XMo
5
(5)
Y
60
61
X
62
Mo
63
XMo
64
Me
65
MeMo
6
(6)
Let us assume that Equations 5 and 6 represent the theoretical models
that are responsible for generating the variance in both the mediator and the
outcome. Accordingly, the model of Equation 4 is a misspecified model. It
then becomes possible to derive the values of the parameters in this
misspecified model in terms of the parameters of Equations 5 and 6.
First, we combine Equations 5 and 6 by substituting for Me in Equation
6 according to Equation 5. This leads to the following result:
Y
60
61
X
62
Mo
63
XMo
64
50
51
X
52
Mo
53
XMo
5
65
50
51
X
52
Mo
53
XMo
5
Mo
6
(6 )
which is given equivalently as:
Y
60
64
50
61
64
51
X
62
64
52
65
50
65 5
Mo
63
64
53
65
51
XMo
65
52
Mo
2
65
53
XMo
2
65 5
6
(6 )
Assuming that X is contrast coded, with an expected value of zero, and
that Mo is normally distributed with a mean of zero and is independent of
X, then it can be shown (Aiken & West, 1991; pp. 177–182; Kenny & Judd,
1984) that the expected covariances of XMo with both Mo
2
and XMo
2
equal
zero. Accordingly, the parameter associated with XMo in the respecified
Equation 6 (
63
64
53
65
51
) must equal its parameter in the
misspecified Equation 4. Accordingly, in the population:
43
63
64
53
65
51
equivalently:
43
63
64
53
65
51
Of course, in any sample of data, the estimated covariances of XMo with
both Mo
2
and XMo
2
will not exactly equal zero and, accordingly, the above
equality will only be approximate.
When Mo is a contrast coded dichotomous variable, then Mo
2
is constant
for all cases (i.e., its variance equals zero). In this case, Equation 6 reduces
to:
Y
60
64
50
65
52
61
64
51
65
53
X
62
64
52
65
50
65 5
Mo
63
64
53
65
51
XMo
64 5
6
(6 )
Accordingly, in this case, the equality holds both in terms of expected
values and in terms of estimated coefficients. In other words, with a
contrast-coded moderator, the following is true of sample estimates:
b
43
b
63
b
64
b
53
b
65
b
51
Received October 11, 2004
Revision received June 20, 2005
Accepted June 20, 2005
863
MEDIATED MODERATION AND MODERATED MEDIATION